If the attribute is unique to only one product, the product is given a score of 1. After this task is completed, Rosch and Mervis suggest that the researcher should review the matrix, and credit any member that clearly and obviously possesses an attribute with the attribute although the attribute may not have been mentioned for the product by any subject. Our findings are further qualified by the use of only one category for the comparison of the five measures. Upon completion of these judgements, family resemblance scores can be computed for the category members. Understanding why a product will be perceived as a member of a particular category is an important issue for consumer researchers and practitioners alike. In the consumer behavior literature, Sujan (1985) used only attributes mentioned by two or more subjects to compute family resemblance. Although his work did not focus on a comparison of his methods with the Rosch et. These findings suggest that users of the family resemblance procedure may be more confident that using the procedures recommended by Rosch does not contribute significant bias to their results. The most prototypical members are those that people tend to think of as the best, truest examples of the category. James Ward, Arizona State University, NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 18 | 1991, Karen Wallach, Emory University, USA
Don Saunders, Arizona State University
Average representation of the "typical" member of a category. The larger the number sharing the attribute, the larger the score. These included being round, crunchy, crisp, divisible into pieces, easily eaten "finger food", appropriate for many occasions, readily transportable, and liked by many people. ----------------------------------------, Advances in Consumer Research Volume 18, 1991 Pages 84-89, THE FAMILY RESEMBLANCE APPROACH TO UNDERSTANDING CATEGORIZATION OF PRODUCTS: MEASUREMENT PROBLEMS, ALTERNATIVE SOLUTIONS, AND THEIR ASSESSMENT. Next, the scores were summed across attributes for each product. First, the researcher develops a list of category members, perhaps by eliciting their names from subjects in a pretest. We can begin to solve the definitional problem by first considering family resemblance and then going beyond it in directions taken by prototype theory. The classical approach to concepts seems to be closed for those who intend to engage rather in descriptive than in prescriptive philosophy. Something else is also required. This could be a problem because the resulting scores may reflect less the attributes salient to subjects than the logic of the judges. If they do, one might conclude that either the judges bias the measures in the expected direction, or improve the scores by in effect "reminding" subjects to accurately describe the stimuli. Seemingly routine as far as small financings go, the transaction soon attracted the attention of the Ontario Securities Commission ("OSC"), not least because both the company and its chi… Nedungadi, Prakash and J. Wesley Hutchinson (1985), "The Prototypicality of Brands: Relationships with Brand Awareness, Preference, and Usage," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. If the judges disagreed about whether a product had an attribute, a third researcher resolved the dispute. The larger the number of attributes a member shares with other category members, and the more widely these attributes are shared with other category members, the higher the family resemblance score. 7 and Wittgensteins Family Resemblance Approach Some see this as a middle ground People may also tend to call a number of other cars "sports cars," but they may tend to regard these as less good, true members of the category. In this procedure, the number of products that shared a particular attribute was first computed, and this score was assigned as a weight to each product having that particular attribute. Perhaps as a response to these potential problems, researchers have over time tried a number of modifications to the family resemblance procedure. The subjects were also asked to rate the prototypicality of the category members on three 010 point scales with endpoints very typical--very atypical, very good example--very poor example, and very representative--very unrepresentative. 4 we show that the family resemblance approach has a number of virtues that the consensus view lacks. Supporting the validity of the typicality rating procedure, the correlation between typicality and production rank, found by past studies to be highly positive (e.g., Ward and Loken 1986, Mervis and Rosch 1981), was .63, p < .05. FR5 was computed to introduce a new factor into the measure, the number of subjects who mentioned an attribute. Mervis, Carolyn and Eleanor Rosch (1981), "Categorization of Natural Objects," Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 89-115. In the next part of the study, subjects' perceptions of the attributes possessed by category members were collected. The most prototypical members are those that people tend to think of as the best, truest examples of the category. We can begin to solve the definitional problem by first considering family resemblance and then going beyond it in directions taken by prototype theory. Thus, in this approach, degree of attribute sharing, or "family resemblance," determines prototypicality. Attributes were written along the right side of the matrix and products along the top. Each of the 20 category members was printed at the top of a page. Second, the results of the study will help researchers assess the comparability of past family resemblance data. b. One approach to understanding the determinants of product categorization that has been applied by a number of researchers (Nedungadi and Hutchinson 1985, Sujan 1985, Ward and Loken 1986, Solomon 1988) is the family resemblance approach initially developed in psychology by Rosch and colleagues (Rosch and Mervis 1975, Mervis and Rosch 1981). In this procedure, the number of products that shared a particular attribute was first computed, and this score was assigned as a weight to each product having that particular attribute. Barasalou, Lawrence (1985), "Ideals, Central Tendency, and Frequency of Instantiation as Determinants of Graded Structure in Categories," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 629-654. Malt and Smith (1984) examined the issue of whether attributes contribute independently to perceived typicality (as assumed by the family resemblance procedure) or whether the correlation among attributes in a category also influences judged typicality. Potential criteria-belief in supernatural beings-communal participation in regular ritualistic activities-belief in an afterlife Despite the utility of the method, close scrutiny suggests some problems and some alternative methods of computing the measure (Loken and Ward 1987). For example, as explained earlier, if eleven products shared an attribute, each product possessing the attribute received a score of eleven. For example, a subject might list the attributes "sweet," "salty," and "coated" for M&M peanut candies but not apples. In this article we present and defend an alternative approach based on the notion of family resemblance. Thus, if eleven products were credited with a particular attribute, each received a score of 11. However, the approach recommended by Rosch and colleagues, although widely adopted, raises questions about whether alternative computational procedures might yield better, or at least different, results. Studies that have applied the family resemblance approach to better understand the determinants of typicality in product categories suggest that the method usually produces scores that correlate highly with alternative measures of category membership, such as typicality ratings, and also yields managerially useful data on what attributes contribute more or less to an item's perception as a member of a category. All told, six clients extended $700,000 of debt financing on the strength of fourteen promissory notes, all of which were secured by a claim against certain assets. Attribute Lists In the next part of the study, subjects' perceptions of the attributes possessed by category members were collected. h�|Z TW���I�61B�VaT�q�Q�Ѹ�"*��"���4Kw��4���/;�Ȧ���c4N�D�21�L����K�$��{o�p(���������k+�V��ֳv��9��j�]h^hm^4���B��xVO�n.T�X�pc�_�\7M_],W����
����.���.�;B������aqɎ��! Subjects were cautioned not to confuse typicality with frequency of encounter or liking, using virtually the same wording as Rosch. Rosch and Mervis (1975) developed a procedure for measuring family resemblance that has been widely cited and used in the psychology literature. The results have implications for both academic and applied students of consumer behavior. 20th century Austrian philosopher; earliest thinker to develop the family resemblances approach, although he used it for language.